The main focus for the week is on empathy. Although every article seemed to define this process differently, empathy is one’s ability to understand and affectively respond to the emotional expression of another. Although the article by Zaki, Bolger, and Ochsner (2008) was a bit of a “no duh;” I thought the methodology and theory was well-lined out. More importantly, I think it highlighted the need to include both parties when studying interactive behavior. It would be especially informative if future neuroscience articles examined dual recordings of empathic responding. On a different note, the Preston et al. (2007) article experimentally confirmed an idea that was given to me in a grade school vocabulary lesson about the difference between empathy and sympathy – namely, that one can only empathize with another when they have in fact experienced something similar. Although I would have liked to see if their individual difference measures had any correlates to the behavioral and neurological data, this article provided insight into the phenomenon of empathy.
On the whole, the articles for the week highlighted the neurological correlates of the empathy process, rather than the effect that empathy has on behavior. Specifically, it was found that sections of the insula and anterior cingulate have interacting connections with limbic areas, such as the amygdala, and regions of the prefrontal cortex, such as the superior temporal and inferior frontal cortices. Because the insula seemed to be such an important cortical structure in our readings for this week, I decided to check out where it is in the brain. Surprisingly this is not as clear as one might think, as there is disagreement about whether or not the insula is its own lobe in the telencephalon (the cerebral cortex) or if it is a portion of the temporal lobe. After examining several images online, it appears that the insula lies at the base of the primary sensory and motor areas and is exterior to the amygdala, which makes sense considering that it has such strong physical and functional links to these areas. In some aside reading, I found it interesting that only humans and great apes may use the insula for higher order cognition. In addition, the insula and the anterior cingulate cortex of humans and great apes are the only areas of the brain that contain von Economo (or spindle) neurons, a unique polar-shaped neuron which may impact a variety of cognitive abilities and have been found to be damaged in Alzheimer’s patients. One should also note that spindle neurons have recently been found in the brains of some species of whales, dolphins, and elephants, which is pretty cool (to me) seeing that great apes, humans, and these animals have the largest brains on earth and all evidence social-emotional bonds between individuals. Recent theorizing even suggests that these neurons may have evolved (relatively recently in the grand scheme of things) to rapidly convey important social information!
When reading the articles for this week, I began to consider the self and other distinctions that we discussed in the beginning of the semester. During that class session, we talked about the importance of medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) activation – specifically, how the mPFC is selectively activated for self-relevant information, compared to activation for close others (e.g., Heatherton et al., 2006). I think it’s important to highlight that without this differential activation, individuals would experience what Lamm, Batson, and Decety (2007) term “empathic overarousal” (p. 43). A follow-up study to examining both self/other distinctions and empathy could examine patients with lesions in the this region to further support the model in which the mPFC plays a role in the ability to distinguish between one’s own emotions and the observed, or simulated, emotions of others. Another interesting parallel between this week and past discussions is the role played by emotion regulation and reappraisal in empathic responses. Specifically, Lamm et al. (2007) showed higher pain intensity and unpleasantness ratings for ineffective treatment stimuli compared to effective treatment stimuli. The authors suggest that this finding may be due to a top-down evaluation mechanism in which participants focus on the long-term outcomes, rather than the immediate experience of pain. An interesting topic to discus may be how these processes interact and which brain regions might be involved.
I read the Zaki et. al. (2008) article first which I felt worked out well. This way I got a little bit of an understanding on empathy research before adding in the neuroscience component. Empathy is considered to have two components. The first is an affective component which is one’s ability to feel the emotions which a target feels. The second component is cognitive and consists of one’s ability to understands and accurately evaluate to cognitive states of another. The correlation between these components has not been observed in most studies which look at these components of empathy. I liked the methods they used in this experiment. Participants first were videotaped describing an emotional event in their life and then watched the videotape and rated their emotions while they were telling the story. A second participant then watched the same videos and rated the emotions of the participants. The perceivers were pretty good at detecting the targets emotions but there was great variance in the accuracy. Perceivers trait affective empathy was not related to accuracy scores but the expressivity scores of the targets was related. There was also an interaction between expressivity and affective empathy. Greater expressivity improved the accuracy scores more for perceivers with high affective empathy scores than those with low scores. This is interesting because this can explain why null results were found in the past. The effect can only be observed when there is high expressivity. It is equally interesting to me why there are null results when the target is low in expressivity. Why to people high in affective empathy struggle just as much as those who are low in affective empathy when expressivity is low? I believe in the result due to its consistency with the past research but it still doesn’t make sense to me. I would almost expect results that say anyone can judge people when they are high in expressiveness but only those high in affective empathy can judge those with low expressiveness accurately.
I next read the Preston article which was a nice jump because it included the neural substrates of cognitive empathy. I had a lot of trouble understanding a lot of this article. Their methods section seemed to jump a lot rather than explaining what was done in order. I believe the anger and fear scenarios which they were told to imagine themselves in were the impersonal events. They rated these scenarios on how well they could relate to these scenarios and the scenarios they could relate to most created similar emotional intensity to personal scenarios. There were no neurological differences between personal and impersonal scenarios. I had a bit of a problem with the scenarios they used. Is reading a little scenario really like putting yourself in the shoes of an actual other person? I don’t think it is. I can feel bad for another person by putting myself in their shoes but I won’t feel anything similar by trying to put myself into a scenario which I know is made up and which no one is actually going through. I think this experiment runs into a lot of the common problems which lab studies will run into when trying to create strong emotions.
I next read the Carr et. al. (2003) article which was a nice summary of the neural correlates of empathy. In this experiment participants were either asked to observe or imitate the emotions displayed by faces which were presented to them. The premotor face area, the dorsal sector of pars opercularis of the inferior frontal gyrus, the superior temporal sulcus, the insula, and the amygdala all showed greater activation during imitation trials than observation trials. The insula may be a critical connection from action representation to emotion because it connects the superior temporal and inferior frontal cortices to the limbic system. A weak connection could explain why some people have trouble with empathy. Going in my typical clinical direction I find this potentially useful. I am not certain if the insula in particular is impaired in psychopaths or even if this impairment would be a cause of effect but it could be interesting to look at.
The last article I read was the Lamm article. I feel like this study may have done a better job than the Preston article in creating a situation where one may feel empathy for another but it still was not enough. I would feel pain watching someone in surgery but watching someone wearing headphones and a turtleneck make faces to aversive noises is not the same. There is no way that participants would believe that the videos were taken in a hospital.Participants were either asked to put themselves in the position of the patient or imagine their feelings. As compared to imagining their feeling there were greater activation in the following areas when they put themselves in the position of the patient. Bilateral insula, left supramarginal gyrus (BA 40), left middle frontal gyrus (Brodmann’s area 9), and in several areas involved in motor control such as the SMA, the right dorsal premotor cortex (lateral BA 6), the putamen, and the caudate nucleus. These results seem consistent with the results found by Carr which points to the importance of the insula.
--
To unsubscribe, reply using "remove me" as the subject.
As everyone knows, this week’s topic is Empathy. Usually our articles tend to use very similar materials and methods, but this time I think all of them were different. Of course, that presents a problem in comparing them, yet many of the articles found significant activation in the same areas which is definitely support for their theories.
Even though the Zaki, Bolger & Oschner (2008) article is the most recent article that we read, I thought I’d start with that. The main point of the article is to suggest that future research use an interactionist approach to empathic accuracy. The authors stated that although past research has had trouble linking the cognitive and affective aspects of empathy, this may be because they have failed to take into account the affect that the target may have on a perceiver’s ability to accurately perceive the target’s emotions. Therefore, their study looked at the perceiver’s trait affective empathy (as measured by the Balanced Emotional Empathy Scale) and empathic accuracy (used as measure of cognitive empathy; correlation between perceivers’ ratings of targets’ feelings and targets’ ratings of their own feelings), as well as the target’s expressivity. Video clips were compiled from a Target Phase in which the participants discussed the 4 most negative and 4 most positive events in their life while being videotaped. Using a sliding 9-point Likert scale, perceivers continuously rated how positive or negative the target was feeling. The results indicated that greater target expressivity improved empathic accuracy of perceivers with high affective empathy more than that of perceivers with low affective empathy. Also, though trait affective empathy had no relationship with empathic accuracy for the least expressive targets, it did predict empathic accuracy when targets were most expressive.
Carr, Iacoboni, Dubeau, Mazziotta & Lenzi (2003) tested an action representation model of empathy. During an fMRI, participants view stimuli (6 emotions of happy, sad, angry, surprise, disgust and fear presented in 3 sets either including only eyes, only mouths or whole faces) presented through magnet-compatible goggles. They were told to either imitate/generate the emotion presented or to observe. The authors predicted and found that activation during observation and imitation of emotions was similar, but there was greater activity during imitation in the premotor areas, inferior frontal cortex, superior temporal cortex, insula and amygdala. The authors concluded that empathy is caused by activating a mechanism of action representation that is associated with the emotion being witnessed.
Lamm, Batson & Decety (2007) stated that the 3 common components of the definitions of empathy in the literature are an affective aspect, cognitive aspect and a monitoring mechanism to discern between self vs. other experiences. In their study, they manipulated perspective (self vs. other) and cognitive appraisals (effective or non-effective treatment). Participants watched a series of video clips of “patients” with a neurological illness that were undergoing a new, painful medical treatment that involved hearing sounds of specific frequencies and amplitudes. Participants were told to imagine themselves or another in the painful situation and notified whether the treatment had been effective or not for that patient. Participants also filled out a series of behavioral measures. The results of perspective-taking found that for the self condition there was greater activity in the left parietal cortex (self-agency area), and the amygdala, insula and aMCC (affective response to threat/pain areas), while for the other-perspective the right parietal cortex was selectively involved. The results for cognitive appraisal found stronger activation in the OFC (emotion reappraisal area), aMCC, insula, and amygdala and higher pain intensity and unpleasantness ratings for the non-effective treatment condition. Overall, I thought this article was well-structured and incorporated a lot of past research findings that supported their own findings.
The Preston et. Al (2007) article specifically looked at cognitive empathy, taking the PET scan after the participant indicated that they were feeling the target emotion. Participants in this study imagined 3 scenarios (a personal experience of own-Personal condition, imagining oneself in an equivalent experience of another- Nonpersonal conditions, & a non-emotional experience of own – Neutral condition). Two experiments were conducted with the difference being that in Experiment 1 participants were instructed to choose 1 of 7 nonpersonal scenarios that participant could relate to the most, and in Experiment 2, that they could relate to the least. Results found no significant differences in activation between personal and nonpersonal conditions in Experiment 1 (most relatable nonpersonal scenario). However, there was more activation in the left inferior temporal, fusiform gyri, and higher-order visual association areas during the nonpersonal imagery in Experiment 2 (least relatable). The authors conclude that people experience “true empathy” when they can relate to the observed situation, but use perspective-taking to achieve empathy in unrelatable situations (“cognitive empathy”).
I feel like the Lamm, Batson and Decety (2007) paper may be one of the
best ones we've read. It's very thorough and has a lot of insightful
information in its discussion. One of the early shortcomings I see to
their study is that they are talking about empathy but only looking at
pain responses, empathy isn't only about pain is it? What about
empathy for other emotions (e.g. positive ones?). Another interesting
point was their selection of only people that seemed to be high in
empathy. How might activation patterns differ in people low in
empathy? I'm not sure if there's a specific basis for expecting
otherwise, but it's always interesting to know as much as we can. Are
the activations simply less or different qualitatively? The other
thing might be to look at brain patterns for people notably low in
empathy, such as persons with antisocial personality disorder.
And those weren't even my discussion questions! (although I'm going to
count them as one of them) I thought it was particularly interesting
how they discuss the differences in empathic concern and pain response
depending on the Self and Other conditions. So we have more empathic
concern in an Other condition, but more pain in the Self condition.
How can we use this information? The authors drop the word
"altruistic" in one form or another a couple of times, but which
experience is more likely to lead to altruistic behavior? The authors
do talk about altruistic motivation, as being related to the empathic
concern more than personal pain, but if people take the more personal
route and imagine that they truly are the person experiencing the
pain; might they be more likely to engage in an altruistic behavior?
The authors' discussion seems so suggest that the empathic concern is
all that matters, but I wonder what everyone else thinks.
The paper by Preston et al. (2007) was also a pretty good read.
However, they mention that one of their analyses may not have yielded
significant results because of a lack of power ... No kidding, your
total sample is less than 20 ppl... low power, eh? *smacks forehead*
I'm finding some things about these articles a little bit confusing.
We are talking about empathy and memory and how it's represented in
the brain, in this study. Models of memory already suggest that when
we retrieve things from long-term memory, we are not, per se,
retrieving a perfect record of what happened at that date and time.
Many of these newer models specify that memory is reconstructive, that
is, when retrieving from long-term memory, we reconstruct the events
of that situation. This is one reason memory can so easily be wrong,
if you fill in a gap with a wrong piece of information, the memory can
be inaccurate. Also, this research shows that when we do this
reconstruction, areas of the brain are activated similarly to how they
are activated when it first occurred. As far as I can tell, this is
synonymous with what we are talking about now. And mirror neurons. So,
when we take the perspective of another, of course similar areas are
activated, they're all perceiving the same damn thing, there just
tends to be these tiny not-even-always-significant areas active that
differentiate self from other.
One of the main points of these studies was that empathizing for
situation to which we can relate yield more self-like activation than
situations we do not relate to as much. I have two questions or
concerns about this result. Is the activation qualitatively different
between situations we do and do not relate to? Or is this some gradual
change in levels of activation in different areas, depending on how
much we can relate to the other person? Second, I think it would be
interesting for another study to manipulate the degree of relation
within-subjects. In other words, instead of showing it in two studies
as they did here, show it in one study. As a follow-up, these
researchers did NOT, per se, show that activation was different
between highly related and not highly related situations, only between
each of these and self perceptions.
I also found it really strange that they found differences in skin
conductance and EMG data between early and late neutral trials, but
only in the second study. Why were these different between the two
studies? Did they talk about this in their discussion? (if so, I
missed it) What's going on with this?
The final paper, by Zaki, Bolger & Ochsner (2008) was interesting as
well. It also used multiple regression, allowing for continuous
predictors, showing that things were not qualitatively different, but
differed across a continuum. The fact that highly expressive targets
were read more accurately is interesting, supporting that "if-then"
model they explain. One thing I wonder is how conscious people are of
these sorts of affects. I think it's likely that what target
expressiveness is a personality trait that manifests itself in
nonverbal actions. It was these nonverbal actions that perceivers were
able to read and use to rate the targets. But how aware were they of
this? Perhaps more generally, how aware are people of how much they
use nonverbal information?
The data of these authors suggests that it's really important to keep
expressivity in mind, rather than simply looking at how well people
can read others. I wonder how activation in the brain would be
different between the people watching themselves and the people
watching someone else. Would activation be essentially the same? Or
would people watching themselves still have activation in those areas
specific to the self?