I couldn't get the newest version of the syllabus to download correctly. Would someone be willing to attach it to an email and send it my way?
Also, are there enough articles for everyone to do 2 or should I plan for some overlap??
--
Lindsay C. Morton, MS
Psychology Department
University at Albany
1400 Washington Avenue
Albany, NY 12222
(973) 534-0530
I couldn't get the newest version of the syllabus to download correctly. Would someone be willing to attach it to an email and send it my way?
Also, are there enough articles for everyone to do 2 or should I plan for some overlap??
On Mon, Apr 19, 2010 at 12:30 PM, Jen Vosilla <jenvo...@gmail.com> wrote:
This covers Biachi-Demiecheli et al with a taste of Aron et al...
Bianchi-Demicheli et al (2006) examined the effects of passionate love on task performance, and found results supporting the idea that priming of a loved-one’s name or passion-related words increases performance on recognition of real words (not fake ones) specifically for those high on the passionate love scale (PLS). While I found their hypothesis and results interesting, I must say that I was disappointed with the lack of distinctive logic in the discussion and the possible erroneous conclusions drawn from the results. Based on their very narrow definition of love and similarly narrow operationalized facilitation on very specific tasks, they for some reason thought this was sufficient to make broad, sweeping claims about love in general and how it must influence all possible tasks one might perform. Only once in the entire article do the authors caveat that there is a distinction between passionate love (based on the PLS they use to measure love in their study) and companionate love (p. 93), both as apply to love between romantic partners and that distinction appears in the methods section, never in the introduction or discussion. Saying “intense love” implies degree of love rather thay qualitative type, but is used repeatedly to refer to passionate love although it is not synonymous .
Most misleading is the authors’ choice of words for their main IV: “women in love” vs “women out of love.” I thought that this categorization was particularly inappropriate because it is suggestive of a control group (e.g., not in love with any romantic partner) rather than a continuum (which is what they were actually measuring, e.g, how much do you feel these particular passionate feeling about your romantic partner) which was absolutely in conflict with their definition of the variable (higher or lower scores on the PLS). Thus despite the fact that both groups “women in love” and “women out of love” might characterize themselves as being “in love” and certainly would characterize themselves as being in a romantic relationship, this was not at all clear in the terms used to discuss this. In addition, the repeated use of these labels was incredibly misleading to readers. Furthermore, this mis-labeling seems to be purposeful, since the discussion and conclusion similarly lead readers to believe that “in love” and “out of love” is then generalizable to all romantic relationships, all types of love, and affects all sorts of tasks. While these conclusions may or may not be correct, the results presented are CERTAINLY not sufficient to draw these conclusions.
Because of their sweeping claims and generalizations, I went back and looked for other problems with sampling data to see if there might be a strong bias due to subject selection, but without the ability to drill down into subjects individual data, the selection seemed reasonably widespread. On a note of practicality, it is hard for me to imagine from their description of feeling “butterflies” and “love at first sight” that members of seriously long term relationships (e.g., married 20+ years) would describe themselves in this way. I suspected that perhaps their claims were based on biased sampling of younger participants, shorter relationships, and stages of love since their conceptual definition of love seemed to be based on what others refer to as early-stage love, however there was no evidence to show that this was definitively the case. While the subject group means did trend in this direction by group, the differences seem relatively minor, and there is no way to further examine whether this is significantly different based on the summary statistics alone. Although the overall means (33 years old versus 35 years old) and ranges (18-56 years old/15 days to 13 years in love versus 23-47 years old/10 days to 30 years in love) seemed acceptably similar, there may have been clustering at higher or lower ends of the spectrum of relationship length (means are not given for relationship length, only range). Even so, I was so unconvinced by their interpretation of the results that I started thinking of various alternative explanations that might account for the effects found, for instance: what if people who score more highly on the PLS are simply more emotionally reactive in general, and therefore react more strongly to passion-related words, which could cause arousal and thus improve performance for these individuals. I just don’t think the authors ruled out a lot of other possible explanations.
It was interesting, however, that in the second study, passionate hobby words were able to elicit facilitation for both groups showing that passion for a person (love) could have similar effects to passion for an activity (hobby). However, consistent with my alternate explanation of higher reactivity, the “in love” group was facilitated more than the “out of love” group.
Neurally, the authors propose that (my addition: at least this subdefinition of) romantic love might be connected with the reward/motivation neural networks (aka the domaminergic system, p. 99; such as the antero-medial caudate nucleus and septum fornix regions, including medial insula, caudate nucleus, putamen/pallidum, ventra tegmenatl area, ACC, hippocampus, frontal gyrus, middle temporal gyrus, right parietal lobe, cerebellum, ) and may depend on dopamine and norepinephrine which are also implicated in “euphoria, craving, addiction, heightened attention, [and] sleeplessness” (p 92). They claim that cognitive facilitation is evidence that love is goal-directed and not just an emotion, but do not flesh out or support this argument. They also note that shorter relationships exhibit higher activations (e.g., stronger “passionate” love) which doesn’t really come into the discussion again despite its relevance to interpretation.
Ultimately, what Bianchi-Demicheli and colleagues did test, and were able to show, is that passionate (priming with lover’s name or passion words) seems to have particular facilitative effects on specified task performance (increased reaction time on lexical decision tasks). And that is ALL they were able to show.
Aron et al (2005) presented a much more nuanced view of romantic love, classifying the “physiological, psychological, and behavioral indices” they were interested in examining as “early-stage romantic love.” I think this distinction is particularly critical given the many types and expressions of love (e.g., maternal, sibling, friend) and even within romantic love, particularly (e.g., passionate versus companionate). Unlinke Bianchi-Demicheli and colleagues, Aron and colleagues were careful to keep their conclusions in context. Aron and colleagues did a much better job stating and proving their case, which was also showing that romantic (early-stage) love can be categorized as goal-directed and activates similar neural systems (particularly the caudate nucleus).
Siegel (2001) presents a theoretical account of the developing brain from the perspective of social-developmental psychology. Attachment is defined as “a basic, in-born, biologically adaptive motivational system that drives the infant to create a few, selective attachments in life.” It is explained that, unlike original conceptualizations of attachment, the individuals to whom the infant becomes attached can be any adult that is present and interacts with the developing infant with no necessity for shared biology or specific gender. The major point of the article seems to be that early attachment experiences with caregivers fundamentally shape the both the physiological connections formed in the brain (i.e., brain specialization and organization) as well as the development of the “mind.” Of interest, the author discusses neural plasticity, or the concept that the brain grows and changes in response to experience throughout the lifespan. At the same time, the argument presented focuses on the importance of early experience when many of these systems are first developing (although it is recognized that attachment classifications and neural circuitry can change later in life).
One of the most interesting points in the paper (in my opinion) deals with the findings that early sensory experiences are not nearly as important to healthy development as interpersonal interactions. Both neurobiological and developmental research support the conclusion that the use of videos, such as Baby Einstein, are not necessary, as only a minimal amount of sensory stimulation is needed for adaptive development in infants. On the other hand, if a caregiver relies solely on such videos and programs rather than collaboratively interacting with their child, the likelihood for suboptimal development increases. Siegel explains that it is through these early collaborative interactions that the infant develops the capacity for self-regulation, theory of mind, and a sense of self. Siegel also touches on research that has found that suboptimal attachment experiences may actually alter the brain’s neuroendocrine response to stress. At the same time, he develops a list of ways to yield secure attachment, which includes collaborative interaction, reflective dialogue, repair of misunderstandings, coherent temporal narratives, and emotional communication. For Seigel, emotional communication seems to be one of the more important factors as this guides the infant’s experiences in his or her preverbal world. Specifically, the importance of non-verbal communication and emotion sharing between the infant and caregiver in the pre-verbal years is believed to impact the development of an array of implicit processes that will remain throughout one’s life. Basically although most individuals do not have an explicit memory of their infancy, that implicit memories are formed during this time. With new research abounding on the impact of implicit processes, this is the first time that I have seen anyone discuss the possible development of such processes (which even if I’m not sure I totally buy – due to lack of empirical evidence- it seems worthy of note). In addition, Siegel points to the importance of the orbitofrontal cortex for the individual’s capacity for response flexibility, or adaptive self-regulation. Of note is the fact that he recognizes that this region is uniquely positioned within the brain so as to have direct communication with limbic areas, other cortex areas, and the brain stem, thus allowing it to functionally regulate such widely distributed input and output in order to achieve response flexibility.
The Lemche et al. (2006) article for this week also focuses on attachment, but their work was designed to capture the neurological correlates of attachment-related reactions. The researchers used a newly validated measure of attachment in which participants were subliminally primed with maternal rejection/separation distress or neutral sentences and reaction times to post-presentation target sentences are recorded. Larger mean reaction times to the target sentence were taken as evidence of insecure attachment. Skin conductance, a known autonomic response indicator, was also measured. The main finding of this work was that the amygdala mediates the autonomic responses associated with attachment insecurity in healthy adult humans. In other words, fMRI evidence demonstrated a positive correlation between bilateral amygdala activation and insecure attachment as well as a positive correlation between bilateral amygdala activation and skin conductance. It was also found that the magnitude of skin conductance (i.e., autonomic response) was positively correlated to insecure attachment in the maternal rejection/separation distress condition. This research adds to a growing body of mammalian studies that suggest that the amygdala is important to attachment.
Gillath, Bunge, Shaver, Wendelken & Mikulincer (2005)
This article looked at attachment-style differences in brain regions associated with emotion, emotion-regulation and memory.
Prior to the fMRI session, participants filled out 3 questionnaires assessing attachment anxiety and avoidance (measured by the Experiences in Close Relationships scale- ECR), neuroticism (a subscale from the Big Five Inventory) and trait anxiety (Spielberger ‘s Trait Anxiety Inventory- STAI). Twenty female participants were chosen that had either a “relatively” high score on one attachment dimension and a score close to the median on the other or low scores on both attachment dimensions (secure attachment). For a study looking for individual differences, the sample size was too low and their only explanation for using only women was “to eliminate gender-related variance”. Is that usually a problem in these types of studies? Also, in choosing participants what constituted a relatively high score or low on the ECR scale? The women also had to be in serious long-term relationships, but this is not defined either. One of my friends considers a relationship that lasts a month as long-term and serious, but I have a different opinion.
The task during the fMRI involved 5 remembered or imagined scenarios that were emotionally neutral (driving alone to Lake Tahoe), neutral but relationship-related (neutral activity with partner), or emotionally negative and relationship-related (conflict with partner, breaking up with partner, and partner dying). For each scenario there were control, think, don’t think and free thinking blocks indicated by colors on a traffic light presented, in which participants pressed a response button during each for varying reasons, when yellow light appeared, shift in thoughts/images, thinking about the forbidden topic and think about formerly forbidden topic, respectively. During the analysis the authors don’t present data from the control and free thinking blocks nor do they assess the 3 emotionally negative and relationship-related scenarios separately, instead always averaging data from all three. What was the point in having 3 of the same condition if the authors didn’t plan to look at them separately, especially considering they vary in how negative they are? My guess is, the results didn’t show any difference between the 3 (which isn’t stated), so they left out predictions about it. Also, I wonder if remembering an actual memory versus imagining the 5 scenarios affected brain activation as the authors did not address this or the intensity of their feelings during the fMRI.
Support for the Main Hypotheses:
1) People with high scores on attachment anxiety have greater activation in emotion-related regions during the emotionally negative, relationship-related scenarios. Results found greater activation in ATP (associated with sadness) and lower activation in OFC (emotion regulation) with a negative correlation between activity in these areas. Authors concluded that people high in attachment anxiety experience high emotional arousal and low ability to down-regulate it
2) People with high scores on attachment anxiety have greater activation in memory-related regions during the emotionally negative, relationship-related scenarios. Results found greater activation in hippocampus (associated with memory retrieval) during the emotionally negative, relationship-related scenarios leading to the conclusion that this increased activation for people high in attachment anxiety is due to recalling a greater number of negative memories.
3) Authors found that attachment avoidant people in comparison to less avoidant people, failed to show as much deactivation in the SCC and LPFC in the Don’t Think block, suggesting that those high in attachment avoidance’s suppression may be less complete or efficient.
Some other criticisms of the article include why they excluded people who scored high on both dimensions of attachment. I could understand the exclusion if they separated the participants into a highly avoidant, highly anxious and secure attachment groups, but they did not and in fact I’d have like to see those comparisons, or even if the highly avoidant and highly anxious were grouped into an insecure attachment group (though according to research that would include those high on both dimensions).
And now for the second article I focused on: (Bartels & Zeki, 2004)
As the title indicates, this article looked at the neural correlates of maternal and romantic love. The authors hypothesized that both maternal and romantic love would share common brain activity due to both having a similar evolutionary origin and biological function, activating brain regions associated with reward (areas rich in oxytocin and vasopressin receptors). For maternal love analysis, 20 mothers (although only 19 included in the analysis) participated in the study, including 2 that were left-handed. Since when is it okay to use lefties? I must have missed the memo that said that there aren’t differences in brain lateralization and even if said memo existed, I would have liked to been told that brain activation in these sinners (I’m lefty so I can say that) did not affect the data analysis. During the fMRI session, participants viewed 4 photographs of either their child, another child that they are acquainted with of the same age, their best friend and one of another person they are acquainted with. Intensity ratings of 8 different emotions were collected after the scan for each of the photographs. Data from Bartels (2000) study was used as the romantic love sample in the analyses. Where did these photographs come from? Maybe it’s just me, but I don’t normally have pictures of child and friend acquaintances and personally I would just print some random pictures I found on Google than actually go take a picture specifically for a study that I’m not getting paid for.
In comparing brain activation in maternal and romantic love participants, the authors found overlapping regions in those associated with the reward system, as well as common deactivation in regions associated with negative emotions, social judgment and ‘mentalizing’. Also, activity in the hypothalamus for romantic love is suggested to be the erotic arousal aspect not found (hopefully) in maternal love. I would have liked to see whether there was a significant difference in levels of activation or deactivation between maternal and romantic love. Both are usually considered strong emotions, but is one more rewarding than the other? Perhaps we can answer the age old question of if you had to sacrifice your husband or your child, which would it be? Speaking of men, future studies should look into similarities and differences between paternal and maternal love. I also agree with David in wondering how a best friend of the opposite gender compares with love for a romantic partner. If as the authors suggest the hypothalamus indicates erotic arousal, then I would predict greater activity for a romantic partner and lower, but still somewhat present, activity for best friends of the opposite gender.