[My] paper ... concludes that the ultimate driver of accuracy is the betting proposition itself rather than any particular trading mechanism: on the one hand, a wager attracts contrarians, which enhances the diversity of opinions that can be aggregated. On the other hand, the mere prospect of reward or loss promotes more objective, less passionate thinking, thereby enhancing the quality of the opinions that can be aggregated. The take-away is that a prediction market is just one of many betting-based methods for aggregating forecasts.
[My] paper ... concludes that the ultimate driver of accuracy is the betting proposition itself rather than any particular trading mechanism: on the one hand, a wager attracts contrarians, which enhances the diversity of opinions that can be aggregated. On the other hand, the mere prospect of reward or loss promotes more objective, less passionate thinking, thereby enhancing the quality of the opinions that can be aggregated. The take-away is that a prediction market is just one of many betting-based methods for aggregating forecasts.
On Apr 23, 2009, Emile Servan-Schreiber wrote:[My] paper ... concludes that the ultimate driver of accuracy is the betting proposition itself rather than any particular trading mechanism: on the one hand, a wager attracts contrarians, which enhances the diversity of opinions that can be aggregated. On the other hand, the mere prospect of reward or loss promotes more objective, less passionate thinking, thereby enhancing the quality of the opinions that can be aggregated. The take-away is that a prediction market is just one of many betting-based methods for aggregating forecasts.I've said (http://www.overcomingbias.com/2006/12/the_80_forecast.html): "I'd guess you can get 80% redict markets offer by using a much simpler solution: collect [forecast] track records." So, yes, the mere prospect that someone may later check your forecasts can encourage you to think more objectively.
When this field was dominated more by academics, they tended more to publish suggested mechanisms for all to see, and to argue for or against mechanisms on the basic of formal proofs or controlled lab experiments. Now that the field is dominated more by private vendors, we see more propriety mechanisms, unavailable for study by critics, and offered without supporting formal proofs or controlled experiments. We instead see more proof by demo and icons; customers are asked to believe that a long list of prior clients, implies their mechanisms are good.Perhaps this is an inevitable progression, but it still saddens me.
I've said (http://www.overcomingbias.com/2006/12/the_80_forecast.html): "I'd guess you can get 80% of the improvement prediction markets offer by using a much simpler solution: collect [forecast] track records." So, yes, the mere prospect that someone may later check your forecasts can encourage you to think more objectively.
It has never been clear how you derive that 80% figure. In my paper, I cite evidence that some non-trading betting schemes can fully match the accuracy of a p /div>
When this field was dominated more by academics, they tended more to publish suggested mechanisms for all to see, and to argue for or against mechanisms on the basic of formal proofs or controlled lab experiments. Now that the field is dominated more by private vendors, we see more propriety mechanisms, unavailable for study by critics, rting formal proofs or controlled experiments. We instead see more proof by demo and icons; customers are asked to believe that a long list of prior clients, implies their mechanisms are good.
Perhaps this is an inevitable progression, but it still saddens me.
You are right that the proliferation of un-tested mechanisms is bothersome. It leaves an open door to snake oil salesmen... That said, selling PMs as easy to design and run in a corporate context also comes close to selling snake oil: "Set it up in minutes! Add a few fun markets! Guaranteed liquidity with our market maker! Free pilot!" Quite a few have been seduced into failure by such simplistic marketese that ignores the complex reality of engaging actual people.
What those of us in the field who don't live by formal proof and controlled experiments have learned is that no matter how formally excellent your market mechanisms is, if not enough people participate then the project is considered a failure. It is considered a failure even if the predictions are accurate, simply because no one will care. Coming up with simpler input mechanisms has helped lower the barrier to participation, which has led to greater acceptance within organizations, and it's really not clear that we've lost any accuracy in the process.
What is needed now is not so much more formal proofs or laboratory experiments on a few undergrads. These are largely irrelevant to what will or won't work in an enterprise or mass-market setting. What is needed is more field studies that embrace reality.
I've said (http://www.overcomingbias.com/2006/12/the_80_forecast.html): "I'd guess you can get 80% of the improvement prediction markets offer by using a much simpler solution: collect [forecast] track records." So, yes, the mere prospect that someone may later check your forecasts can encourage you to think more objectively.
fully match the accuracy of a prediction market.
You are right that the proliferation of un-tested mechanisms is bothersome. It leaves an open door to snake oil salesmen... That said, selling PMs as easy to design and run in a corporate context also comes close to selling snake oil: "Set it up in minutes! Add a few fu uidity with our market maker! Free pilot!" Quite a few have been seduced into failure by such simplistic marketese that ignores the complex reality of engaging actual people.
What those of us in the field who don't live by formal proof and controlled experiments have learned is that no matter how formally excellent your market mechanisms is, if not enough people participate then the project is considered a failure. It is considered a failure even if the predictions are accurate, simply because no one will care. Coming up with simpler input mechanisms has helped lower the barrier to participation, which has led to greater acceptance within organizations, and it's really not clear that we've lost any accura /div>
What is needed now is not so much more formal proofs or laboratory experiments on a few undergrads. These are largely irrelevant to what will or won't work in an enterprise or mass-market setting. What is needed is more field studies that embrace reality.